Sign Language



SIGN LANGUAGE







Compiled by:
1.                 Risca Nur Kafidah       (13020230058)
2.                 Munawaroh                  (13020230023)




PREFACE


Alhamdulillah, we thank to Allah Subhanahu Wa Ta’ala who always gives us his blessing and ease in processing this paper. So, we are able to finish it, as the duty of linguistic subject, in time.
            We also thank to all of the participants who have arranged the materials in this paper. And we don’t forget to say thank you very much to Miss Ayu Fatmawati, M.Pd who has supported us.
However, we realize that there are still many shortcomings in this paper. Therefore, we enthusiastically welcome the objective criticism and constructive suggestion for the improvement of this paper. Finally, we do hope that this paper will be useful for readers.




Kediri

Table of Content


Preface
Table of Content
Chapter I: Intro to Sign Language
Chapter II: American Sign Language
Chapter III: The Acquisition of Sign Language
Chapter IV: The Linguistic Structure of Sign Language
Reference

CHAPTER I
Intro to Sign Language

          Only the profoundly brain damaged, psychotic, or abused fail to acquire or maintain language abilities. Humans acquire language even in the presence of deafness, muteness, blindness, and many forms of brain damage, depressed emotional states, and serious psychological conditions. This paper deals with the linguistic abilities of deaf people.
            Many people consider that deaf people are mute and unintelligent. They also believe that without speech a person cannot form complex ideas and cannot efficiently communicate with others. However, this belief is the false notion. Human facility for language is not dependent on either speech or hearing.
            We know that speech is one delivery system for language. But the auditory-vocal method of delivery is not the only channel on which linguistic information can be carried and received. Language can also be conveyed through the manual-visual channel by the use of sign language.
            A sign language (also signed language or simply signing) is a language which uses manual communication and body language to convey meaning, as opposed to acoustically conveyed sound patterns. This can involve simultaneously combining handshapes, orientation and movement of the hands, arms or body, and facial expressions to fluidly express a speaker's thoughts. They share many similarities with spoken languages (sometimes called "oral languages", which depend primarily on sound), which is why linguists consider both to be natural languages, but there are also some significant differences between signed and spoken languages.
Wherever communities of deaf people exist, sign languages develop. Signing is also done by persons who can hear, but cannot physically speak. Sign languages show the same linguistic properties and use the same language faculty as do spoken languages.
Although some people have contended that signing is a universal language, it is not. Those who argue that it is universal assert that sign language is easy for any one to understand because it is iconic. An iconic sign is picturelike, it is a mimetic representation of some phenomenon. But iconic signs are arbitrary because they belong to particular culture. For example, a sign that vaguely looks like a tree may look like a tree only to the people of a specific signing community. Other signing communities may use a different sign to indicate tree. So, a sign can still be arbitrary even if it is iconic.
 

            Sign languages can be more iconic than speech because signers use three-dimensional space. A signer can draw a picture in the air that might come close to illustrating what the sign represents. Then there are many sign languages used by deaf people. Some of them are Australian Sign Language, British Sign Language, Danish Sign Language, Chinese Sign Language, Taiwan Sign Language, Israeli Sign Language, and Thai Sign Language. One of the most researched of any signed language has been American Sign Language (ASL).


CHAPTER II
American Sign Language
 

What is American Sign Language?
American Sign Language (ASL) is a complete, complex language that employs signs made by moving the hands combined with facial expressions and postures of the body. It is the primary language of many North Americans who are deaf and is one of several communication options used by people who are deaf or hard-of-hearing.

Where did ASL originate?
The exact beginnings of ASL are not clear, but some suggest that it arose more than 200 years ago from the intermixing of local sign languages and French Sign Language (LSF, or Langue des Signes Française). Today’s ASL includes some elements of LSF plus the original local sign languages, which over the years have melded and changed into a rich, complex, and mature language. Modern ASL and modern LSF are distinct languages and, while they still contain some similar signs, can no longer be understood by each other’s users.

How does ASL compare with spoken language?
In spoken language, words are produced by using the mouth and voice to make sounds. But for people who are deaf (particularly those who are profoundly deaf), the sounds of speech are often not heard, and only a fraction of speech sounds can be seen on the lips. Sign languages are based on the idea that vision is the most useful tool a deaf person has to communicate and receive information.
ASL is a language completely separate and distinct from English. It contains all the fundamental features of language—it has its own rules for pronunciation, word order, and complex grammar. While every language has ways of signaling different functions, such as asking a question rather than making a statement, languages differ in how this is done. For example, English speakers ask a question by raising the pitch of their voice; ASL users ask a question by raising their eyebrows, widening their eyes, and tilting their bodies forward.
Just as with other languages, specific ways of expressing ideas in ASL vary as much as ASL users do. In addition to individual differences in expression, ASL has regional accents and dialects. Just as certain English words are spoken differently in different parts of the country, ASL has regional variations in the rhythm of signing, form, and pronunciation. Ethnicity and age are a few more factors that affect ASL usage and contribute to its variety.

  
CHAPTER III
The Acquisition of Sign Language

Nowhere is the ‘natural laboratory’ metaphor more appropriate than in the field of sign language acquisition. This area of inquiry offers a novel and especially revealing vantage point from which to address weighty questions about the human capacity for language. Research has shown, for example, that children acquire sign language without instruction, just as hearing children acquire spoken language, and according to the same timetable (Newport and Meier, 1985). These findings lend more credence to the view, established by linguistic research on the adult system, that languages in the two modalities share a significant amount of cognitive territory; children come equipped for the task of acquiring language in either modality equally.
Studies have also shown that signing children attend to grammatical properties, decomposing and over-generalizing them as they advance through the system, sometimes even at the expense of the iconic properties inherent in that system.
Parents are often the source of a child’s early acquisition of language, but for children who are deaf, additional people may be models for language acquisition. A deaf child born to parents who are deaf and who already use ASL will begin to acquire ASL as naturally as a hearing child picks up spoken language from hearing parents. However, for a deaf child with hearing parents who have no prior experience with ASL, language may be acquired differently. In fact, nine out of 10 children who are born deaf are born to parents who hear. Some hearing parents choose to introduce sign language to their deaf children. Hearing parents who choose to learn sign language often learn it along with their child. Surprisingly, children who are deaf can learn to sign quite fluently from their parents, even when their parents might not be perfectly fluent themselves.


CHAPTER IV
The Linguistic Structure of Sign Language

Hearing people use gesture, pantomime, and facial expression to augment spoken language. Naturally, the ingredients of these forms of expression are available to sign languages too. The apparent familiarity of the raw material that contributes to the formation of sign languages has led many a native observer to the mistaken assumption that sign languages are actually simple gesture systems. However, instead of forming an idiosyncratic, ancillary system like the one that accompanies speech, these basic ingredients contribute to a primary linguistic system in the creation of a sign language, a system with many of the same properties found in spoken languages. In fact, linguistic research has demonstrated that there are universal organizing principles that transcend the physical modality, subsuming spoken and signed languages alike.

THE PHONOLOGY OF SIGN LANGUAGE
William Stokoe (1960) demonstrated that the signs of American Sign Language (ASL) are not gestures: they are not holistic icons. Instead, Stokoe (1960) showed that they are comprised of a finite list of contrastive meaningless units like the phonemes of spoken languages. These units combine in constrained ways to create the words of the language. While some differences have turned up among different sign languages in their phonological inventories and constraints, there are many common properties, and the generalizations presented here hold across sign languages, unless otherwise indicated.
Stokoe established three major phonological categories: handshape, location, and movement. Each specification within each of the three major categories was treated as a phoneme in Stokoe’s work. Later researchers accepted these categories, but proposed that the specifications within each category function not as phonemes but as phonological features.
The ASL signs SICK and TOUCH, illustrated in Figure 1, have the same handshape and the same straight movement. They are distinguished by location only: the location for SICK is the head, while the location for TOUCH is the non-dominant hand. Minimal pairs such as this one, created by differences in one feature only, exist for the features of handshape and movement as well. While the origins of these and other (but certainly not all) signs may have been holistic gestures, they have evolved into words in which each formational element is contrastive but meaningless in itself.
Figure 1. ASL minimal pair distinguished by location feature.
a. SICK and b. TOUCH

Two other defining properties of phonological systems exist in sign languages as well: constraints on the combination of phonological elements, and rules that systematically alter their form. One phonological constraint on the form of a (monomorphemic) sign concerns the set of two-handed signs. If both hands are involved, and if both hands also move in producing the sign (unlike TOUCH above, in which only one hand moves), then the two hands must have the same handshape, and the same (or mirror) location and movement.
An example is DROP, pictured in Figure 2b: both hands move, and they are identical in all other respects as well. The second defining property, changes in the underlying phonological form of a sign, is exemplified by handshape assimilation in compounds. In the lexicalized ASL compound, MIND+DROP=FAINT, the handshape of the first member, MIND, undergoes total assimilation to the handshape of the second member, DROP, as shown in Figure 2.
         +        =     
 Figure 2. Hand configuration assimilation in the ASL compound,
a. MIND + b. DROP = c. FAINT
Stokoe believed that handshapes, locations, and movements co-occur simultaneously in signs, an internal organization that differs from the sequentiality of consonants and vowels in spoken language. Liddell (1984) took exception to that view, showing that there is phonologically significant sequentiality in this structure. Sandler (1989) further refined that position, arguing that the locations (L) and movement (M) within a sign are sequentially ordered, while the hand configuration (HC) is autosegmentally associated to these elements -- typically, one hand configuration (i.e., one handshape with its orientation) to a sign, as shown in the representation in Figure 3.
The first location of the sign TOUCH in Figure 1b, for example, is a short distance above the non-dominant hand; the movement is a straight path; and the second location is in contact with the non-dominant hand.
                                                                    L            M           L
Figure 3. The canonical form of a sign (Sandler, 1989)
Under assimilation, as in Figure 2, the HC of the second member of the compound spreads regressively to the first member in a way that is temporally autonomous with respect to the Ls and Ms, manifesting the auto segmental property of stability (Goldsmith, 1979). The sequential structure of signs is still a good deal more limited than that of words in most spoken languages, however, usually conforming to this canonical LML form even when the signs are morphologically complex (Sandler, 1993).

THE MORPHOLOGY OF SIGN LANGUAGE
All established sign languages studied to date, like the overwhelming majority of spoken languages, have complex morphology. First, as shown above in Figure 2., compounding is very common. In addition, some sign languages have a limited number of sequential affixes. For example, Israeli Sign Language (ISL) has a derivational negative suffix, similar in meaning to English –less, that was grammaticalized from a free word glossed NOT-EXIST. This suffix has two allomorphs, depending on the phonological form of the base, illustrated in Figure 4. If the base is two-handed, so is the suffix, while one-handed bases trigger the one-handed allomorph of the suffix.


a.       IMPORTANT-NOT-EXIST (without importance)

 
b. INTERESTING-NOT-EXIST (without interest)
Figure 4. Allomorphs of an ISL suffix.

Sign languages typically have a good deal of complex morphology, but most of it is not sequential like the examples in Figures 3 and 4. Instead, signs gain morphological complexity by simultaneously incorporating morphological elements (Fischer and Gough, 1978). The prototypical example, first described in detail in ASL (Padden, 1988) but apparently found in all established sign languages, is verb agreement. This inflectional system is prototypical not only because of the simultaneity of structure involved, but also because of its use of space as a grammatical organizing property.
The system relies on the establishment of referential loci – points on the body or in space that refer to referents in a discourse – which might be thought of as the scaffolding of the system. In Figure 5, loci for first person and third person are established.

Figure 5. Referential loci. a. First person. b. Third person.

In the class of verbs that undergoes agreement, the agreement markers correspond to referential loci established in the discourse. Through movement of the hand from one locus to the other, the subject is marked on the first location of the verb, and the object on the second. Figure 6a shows agreement for the ASL agreeing verb, ASK, where the subject is first person and the object is third person. Figure 6b shows the opposite: third person subject and first person object. The requirement that verb agreement must refer independently to the first and last locations in a sign was one of the motivations for Liddell’s (1984) claim that signs have sequential structure.
Figure 6. Verb agreement. a. ‘I ask him/her.’ b. ‘s/he asks me.’
Although each verb in Figure 6 includes three morphemes, each still conforms to the canonical LML form shown in Figure 3. The agreement markers are encoded without sequential affixation. Sign language verb agreement is a linguistic system, crucially entailing such grammatical concepts as co-reference, subject and object, singular and plural. It is also characterized by sign language-specific properties, such as the restriction of agreement to a particular class of verbs (Padden, 1998), identified mainly on semantic grounds (Meir, 2002).
Another productive inflectional morphological system found across sign languages is temporal and other aspectual marking, in which the duration of Ls and Ms, the shape of the movement path, or both may be altered, and the whole form may be reduplicated, to produce a range of aspects, such as durational, continuative, and iterative (Klima and Bellugi 1979). This system has templatic characteristics, lending itself to an analysis that assumes CV-like LM templates and nonlinear associations of the kind McCarthy (1981) proposed for Semitic languages (Sandler, 1989, 1990).
Figure 4 demonstrated that some limited sequential affixation exists in sign languages. However, the most common form of sign language words by far, whether simple or complex, is LML (setting aside gemination of Ls and Ms in the aspectual system, which adds duration but not segmental content). In fact, even lexicalized compounds such as the one shown in Figure 2 often reduce to this LML form. If movement (M) corresponds to a syllable nucleus in sign language, as Perlmutter (1992), Brentari (1998), and others have argued, then it appears that monosyllabicity is ubiquitous in ASL (Coulter, 1982) and in other sign languages as well. In the midst of a morphological system with many familiar linguistic characteristics – e.g., compounding, derivational morphology, inflectional morphology, allomorphy – we see in the specific preferred monosyllabic form of sign language words a clear modality effect (Sandler & Lillo-Martin, in press).
No overview of sign language morphology would be complete without a description of the classifier subsystem. This subsystem is quintessentially ‘sign language’, exploiting the expressive potential of two hands forming shapes and moving in space, and molding it into a linguistic system (Supalla, 1986; Emmorey, 2003). Sign languages use classifier constructions to combine physical properties of referents with the spatial relations among them and the shape and manner of movement they execute. In this subsystem, there is a set of hand shapes which classify referents in terms of their size and shape, semantic properties, or other characteristics, in a classificatory system that is reminiscent of verbal classifiers found in a variety of spoken languages (Senft, 2002).
These hand shapes are the classifiers that give the system its name. An example of a classifier construction is shown in Figure 7. It describes a situation in which a person is moving ahead, pulling a recalcitrant dog zigzagging behind.

Figure 7. classifier construction in ASL
What is unusual about this subsystem is that each formational element – the hand shape, the location, and the movement – has meaning. That is, each has morphological status. This makes the morphemes of classifier constructions somewhat anomalous, since sign language lexicons are otherwise built of morphemes and words in which each of these elements is meaningless and has purely phonological status. Furthermore, constraints on the co-occurrence of these elements in other lexical forms do not hold on classifier constructions. In Figure 7, for example, the constraint on interaction of the two hands described in the section on phonology is violated. Zwitserlood (2003) suggests that each hand in such classifier constructions is articulating a separate verbal constituent, and that the two occur simultaneously – a natural kind of structure in sign language and found universally in them, but one that is inconceivable in spoken language. Once again, sign language presents a conventionalized system with linguistic properties, some familiar from spoken languages, and some modality-driven.

SYNTAX OF SIGN LANGUAGE
As in other domains of linguistic investigation, the syntax of sign languages displays a large number of characteristics found universally in spoken languages. A key example is recursion – the potential to repeatedly apply the same rule to create sentences of ever increasing complexity – argued to be the quintessential linguistic property setting human language apart from all other animal communication systems (Hauser, Chomsky, and Fitch, 2002). Specifically, through embedding or conjoining, recursion can result in sentences of potentially infinite length. These two different ways of creating complex sentences have been described and distinguished from one another in American Sign Language. For example, a process that tags a pronoun that is co-referential with the subject of a clause onto the end of a sentence may copy the first subject in a string only if the string contains an embedded clause, but not if the second clause is coordinate (Padden 1988). In example (1), the subscripts stand for person indices marked through verb agreement, and INDEX is a pointing pronominal form, here a pronoun copy of the matrix subject, MOTHER. (These grammatical devices were illustrated in Figures 5 and 6 above.)

(1) a. MOTHER SINCE iPERSUADEj SISTER jCOMEi iINDEX
‘My mother has been urging my sister to come and stay here, she (mother) has.’
b. * iHITj jINDEX TATTLE MOTHER iINDEX.
‘I hit him and he told his mother, I did.’

The existence of strict constraints on the relations among nonadjacent elements and their interpretation is a defining characteristic of syntax. A different category of constraints of this general type concerns movement of constituents from their base generated position, such as the island constraints first put forward by Ross (1967) and later subsumed by more general constraints. One of these is the WH island constraint, prohibiting the movement of an element out of a clause with an embedded WH question. The sentence, Lynn wonders [what Jan thinks] is okay, but the sentence *It’s Jan that Lynn wonders [what __ thinks] is ruled out. Lillo-Martin (1991) demonstrated that ASL obeys the WH island constraint with the sentences shown in example (2). Given the relative freedom of word order often observed in sign languages such as ASL, it is significant that this variability is nevertheless restricted by universal syntactic constraints.

(2) a. PRO DON’T-KNOW [“WHAT” MOTHER LIKE].
‘I don’t know what Mom likes.’
_______t
b. MOTHER, PRO DON’T KNOW [“WHAT” ___ LIKE].
* ‘As for Mom, I don’t know what likes.’

The line over the word MOTHER in (2b) indicates a marker that is not formed with the hands, in this case a backward tilt of the head together with raised eyebrows, marking the constituent as a topic (t) in ASL. There are many such markers in sign languages, which draw from the universal pool of idiosyncratic facial expressions and body postures available to all human communicators and which become organized into a grammatical system in sign languages.

A GRAMMAR OF THE FACE
When language is not restricted to manipulations of the vocal tract and to auditory perception, it is free to recruit any parts of the body capable of rapid, variegated articulations that can be readily perceived and processed visually, and so it does. All established sign languages that have been investigated use non-manual signals – facial expressions and head and body postures -- grammatically. These expressions are fully conventionalized and their distribution is systematic.
Early research on ASL showed that certain facial articulations, typically of the mouth and lower face, function as adjectival and as manner adverbials, the latter expressing such meanings as ‘with relaxation and enjoyment’, and ‘carelessly’ (Liddell, 1980). Other sign languages have been reported to use lower face articulations in similar ways. The specific facial expressions and their associated meanings vary from sign language to sign language. Figure 8 gives examples of facial expressions of this type in ASL, ISL and British Sign Language, resp.

Figure 8. a. ASL ‘with relaxation and enjoyment b. ISL ‘carefully’, and c. BSL ‘exact’

A different class of facial articulations, particularly of the upper face and head, predictably co-occur with specific constructions, such as yes/no questions, WH questions, and relative clauses in ASL and in many other sign languages as well. Examples from ISL shown in Figure 9a illustrate a yes/no question (raised brows, wide eyes, head forward), 9b a WH question (furrowed brows, head forward), and 9c the facial expression systematically associated in that language with information designated as ‘shared’ for the purpose of the discourse (squinted eyes). While some of these facial articulations may be common across sign languages (especially those accompanying yes/no and WH questions), these expressions are not iconic. Some researchers have proposed that they evolved from more general affective facial expressions associated with emotions. In sign languages, however, they are grammaticalized, and formally distinguishable from the affective kind that signers, of course, also use (Reilly, McIntire, and Bellugi, 1990).
  
Figure 9. ISL a. yes/no question, b. WH question, and c. ‘shared information’

Observing that non-manual signals of the latter category often co-occur with specific syntactic constructions, Liddell (1980) attributed to them an expressly syntactic status in the grammar of ASL, a view that other researchers have adopted and expanded (e.g., Petronio and Lillo-Martin 1997; Neidle, Kegl, MacLaughlin, Bahan, & Lee, 2000).
A competing view proposes that they correspond to intonational tunes (Reilly, McIntire and Bellugi, 1990) and participate in a prosodic system (Nespor and Sandler, 1999). Wilbur (2000) presents evidence that non-manuals convey many different kinds of information -- prosodic, syntactic, and semantic. A detailed discussion can be found in Sandler and Lillo-Martin (in press).

To get the ppt, click here !

References
Rowe, Bruce M. And Levine, Diane P. 2011. A Concise Introduction to Linguistics. New Jersey. Prentice Hall.
Sandler, Wendy. To Appear in Encyclopedia of Language and Linguistics. The University of Haifa.


Tags
Previous article
Next article
This Is The Oldest Page

0 Response to "Sign Language"

Posting Komentar

Kode Iklan Atas Artikel

Kode Iklan Tengah Artikel 1

Kode Iklan Tengah Artikel 2

Kode Iklan Bawah Artikel